Tactical Synergy is Proud to Present Captain Kurt Frisz of the St Louis Missouri Police Department
On August 9th, 2014 - Unarmed 18 year old Michael Brown was shot and killed by a Ferguson Missouri police office. This started 17 days of riots which were broad cast around the world. For the first time Capt Kurt Frisz will debrief the incident. Captain Frisz was present during the riot and part of the Command structure. Captain Frisz will debrief some of the following topics...

Captain Kurt Frisz has been with the St. Louis County (MO) Police Department for 29 years. He served over 17 years to the departments Tactical Operations Unit, 7 years as the units’ commander. During this time, he has commanded over 100 hostage/barricade situations, and has been involved in numerous civil unrest situations.

Synopsis

On August 9th, 2014 - Unarmed 18 year old Michael Brown was shot and killed by a Ferguson Missouri police office. This started 17 days of riots which were broad cast around the world. For the first time Capt Kurt Frisz will debrief the incident. Captain Frisz was present during the riot and part of the Command structure. Captain Frisz will debrief some of the following topics:

  • The facts surrounding the shooting of Michael Brown
  • The use of riot squads
  • The use of SWAT
  • Leadership issues
  • Media placing officers and citizens in danger
  • Politics, including the involvement of the Whitehouse
  • The perceived militarization of policing in North America

Who Should Attend

Swat; Riot control units; Commanders; Law enforcement administrators; Law enforcement trainers; Use of force evaluators; K-9, anyone interested in officer safety and enhancing their knowledge.

Registration
Date:Monday November 17, 2014 - 1000 hours - 1600 hours
Location:Venue is TBA; Location Abbotsford, BC
Cost:$100 prior to November 1st, 2014; $135 after November 1st, 2014
To Register:Contact Matt Sekela 604-845-7928 or E mail matt@tacticalsynergy.ca
Read more...

Sgt Kevin Barret to Speak in Abbotsford
Officer involved shooting debrief.

Tactical Synergy is proud to announce Sgt Kevin Barrett, the President and Founder of the International Tactical Officers Association

Tactical Synergy is proud to announce Sgt Kevin Barrett, the President and Founder of the International Tactical Officers Association, will be in Abbotsford on June 18th, 2012 to speak on the following:

For US Law Enforcement, 2011 was very disturbing due to an increasing trend of officer involved shootings and officer deaths. This presentation will debrief one of those deaths. A police officer employed by a larger than average US police department was killed in action while assisting the U.S. Marshal’s Service on a felony arrest warrant at the suspects home. This officer’s partner, a K9, was seriously wounded during the incident but survived. The suspect also died. This officer was a K9 handler and a SWAT Sniper. This incident debrief will attempt to put into prospective what really happened prior to, during and after this fatal incident. The call to a service, the raid planning, the inner workings of the scene and the tactical response, command and control and crime scene issues will be discussed. The offender’s criminal history, a timeline of events along with on scene photos and radio traffic will be presented. The outcome will be a discussion on lessons learned and what steps could be taken to prevent this type of incident happening again.

Location: 32470 Haida Drive , Abbotsford, BC, V2T 5A6
Date: June 18th,2012
Time: 0800-1600 hours
Cost: $60.00 including HST
Seating will be limited so please try and register early

To register contact Matt Sekela at 604-226-6288 or at mrsekela@shaw.ca

Read more...

Force Science News: Transmission 164

Training note: The next Force Science Certification Course is scheduled for February 14-18, 2011 in San Jose, CA. To register please e-mail training@forcescience.org. Remaining seats are very limited so please wait for confirmation before making travel arrangements.

New Force Science study results: Prone suspects with hidden hands more dangerous than imagined

The latest study by the Force Science Institute has produced 2 surprising findings of importance to trainers, street officers, and police attorneys:

  1. Some suspects lying flat with hands hidden under chest or waist can produce and fire a gun at an approaching officer faster than any human being on earth can react to defend himself;
  2. The angle sometimes advocated as the safest for approaching a prone subject appears, in fact, to be potentially the most dangerous.

In testing 5 different angles of weapon exposure and attack, FSI researchers discovered that the overall average time that elapses between the instant a prone suspect's first movement can be seen and the discharge of his pointed weapon is less than 2/3 of a second.

One subject in one of the firing postures monitored was able to move so fast that the gun in his hand could not be detected until the moment it discharged. The fastest subjects produced the weapon from under their chest and fired it upward and ahead--the line of approach taught by some trainers as being the most protective for officers.

One trainer who witnessed the testing exclaimed: "Wow! I knew suspects could be fast, but I didn't know they could be that fast!"

"This study is the first of its kind," lead researcher Dr. Bill Lewinski told Force Science News, "and it scientifically establishes that the desperate urgency officers often feel to control a prone subject's hands is fully justified.

"If hidden hands are not controlled immediately and the suspect is armed and decides to shoot, an officer is likely faced with an insurmountable challenge to react fast enough to prevent what could be a fatal attack."

RESEARCH MOTIVATION. Common street sense dictates that a live suspect lying on his belly with 1 or both hands hidden under his body poses a potential threat because of his possible access to a concealed weapon. However, Lewinski points out, "all training and tactics for dealing with this real-life field problem have been based on anecdotal experience, impulse, and supposition, not on any scientific foundation."

Moreover, in recent years a number of controversial, high-profile encounters have been captured on news video, showing officers using what appeared to be extraordinary force to expose downed suspects' hidden hands during capture and arrest.

"Media critics and other civilians, including jurors and force review board members, seemed unable to understand the officers' sense of urgency in some of these cases," says Lewinski, FSI's executive director. "Strikes with batons or flashlights delivered by officers trying to gain control of resistant suspects' hands were sometimes interpreted as malicious outbreaks of rage and vindictiveness.

"It became clear that we needed to scientifically explore the threat level presented by prone suspects with hidden hands because of the significant legal, training, and survival implications inherent in this subject."

Sgt. Craig Allen of the Hillsboro (OR) PD, the on-site coordinator for the resulting FSI study, put it this way: "Let's have the facts. Once we know for certain what we're dealing with, we can understand, explain, and train."

TESTING SET-UP. After some preliminary testing at FSI headquarters in Minnesota and at the Northeast Wisconsin Technical College to refine methods, Lewinski and his research crew last February performed a 4-day series of rigorous experiments in Oregon with the help of Hillsboro PD's training unit.

One at a time, 39 volunteers--a mixture of male and female LEOs and college students, ranging in age from 19 to 32 and with varied fitness and agility levels--proned out on mats on the floor of a vacant commercial building. Each held a .22-cal., J-frame S&W revolver loaded with black-powder blanks under their chest or waist.

Each volunteer fired 25 rounds, producing the gun and shooting 1 round as fast as possible 5 different times in each of 5 different directions: from the chest up and ahead, to the left rear, and to the right rear, and from waist level to the left rear and to the right rear. Each was told to shoot as if trying to hit an officer center-mass approaching from those various directions at a distance of about 10 feet.

Three high-definition video cameras positioned at 3 different angles filmed the action. These time-coded tapes were then synced and meticulously analyzed under the direction of safety-management researcher and doctoral candidate Madeleine Gonin at the Ergonomics Laboratory at Indiana University.

Some of the participants were also filmed by the Canadian Discovery Channel. CLICK HERE to watch the clip."Each of the subjects moved in a somewhat different way, depending on what seemed most natural and fastest to them," Lewinski says.

SURPRISING FINDINGS. From Gonin's analysis of various elements in nearly a gigabyte of video footage, 2 measurements are the most significant, Lewinski explains.

One is the amount of time that elapses from the moment a subject starts his or her first, detectable pre-attack movement (usually a shifting of feet or hips) until the gun discharges. The other is the time from when any part of the gun is first visible until it fires; that is, from the time "something" from under the suspect's body--not even yet identifiable as a weapon--is first captured in a camera frame.

"All the time lapses recorded are startlingly fast--much faster than we imagined before the experiments," Lewinski says.

Specifically:

CLICK HERE to see these findings presented in a bar graph format.

DISTURBING INTERPRETATION. "It's important for officers to know how quickly an attack can unfold, because in terms of reaction time to sudden threats, a targeted officer is very likely to be significantly behind the curve," Lewinski says. "This is consistent with findings from other Force Science time-and-motion studies."

He points to the fastest times in which the research subjects were able to fire after some part of their gun first became visible. For some, there was no time gap; the gun could not be seen until it discharged. At most, only 1/10 of a second elapsed. Even the averages, lengthened by inclusion of the slowest shooters, ranged between ? second and less than half a second.

"There is not a human being in the world who can react before the discharge in those time frames, even if they are expecting a threat and have their gun up and ready!" Lewinski declares. "Even before the object coming into view can be recognized as a gun, a shot is off."

Nor can an approaching officer expect to be alerted by a suspect's pre-attack movement in time to preempt the threat. Even the slowest average time from initial movement to discharge is less than ? second. "Seeing a suspect's feet or hips start to shift to provide a physical base for bringing a gun out is of virtually no value in a swift attack," Lewinski says. "There's not enough time to comprehend what's happening and react."

Most surprising, Lewinski says, were the results when test subjects produced a gun from under their chest and fired to the front and up at about a 45-degree angle.

"Some trainers and officers believe that approaching a downed suspect toward the head provides the least vulnerability because lifting the torso up to shoot takes more effort," Lewinski says. "But ironically the fastest shooting times were achieved by subjects attacking toward that direction. In reality, the chest can be lifted and a gun pushed out with very little dynamic movement.

"Average times both from motion to discharge (0.52 seconds) and from appearance to discharge (0.25 seconds) are lowest in that position. And in the worst case from an officer's perspective, the gun is not at all visible until the instant it fires (0.00 seconds)."

LEGAL & TRAINING IMPLICATIONS. The scientific documentation of how quickly deadly threats can materialize from prone suspects could be helpful in explaining to force reviewers why officers sometimes feel compelled to use vigorous physical tactics in gaining control of hidden hands, Lewinski believes.

The legal impact will be discussed in greater detail by Capt. Scott Sargent of the LAPD, an attorney and certified Force Science Analyst, as part of an official paper on the study to be published by the researchers in a peer-reviewed professional journal. We'll advise you when this is available, expected to be in spring 2011.

As to tactical training implications, Lewinski shares a couple of preliminary observations:

  1. In parsing the study data, it appears that prone suspects tend to be slowest in delivering gunfire when they are shooting toward the rear on the side opposite their gun hand. Thus in this study, in which most participants were right-handed, the slowest time averages from motion or weapon appearance to discharge occurred when subjects were shooting to the left rear with a gun hidden at waist level.

    "This is because they had to turn more to free the gun arm from under their body," Lewinski explains. "Some subjects, in fact, had to roll almost onto their back before being able to shoot. Consequently, approaching toward a prone suspect's feet may be marginally safer--if anything can be considered safe in coming up to a downed suspect whose hands are hidden."

  2. Keeping the suspect uncertain as to the approaching officer's location may be the best tactic for buying reaction time or forestalling an attempted attack.

    "This may require deviation from the normal contact/cover approach," Lewinski explains. "The contact officer, who normally would be giving commands, can remain silent while the cover officer, ideally behind some protective barrier, issues verbal directions. This may allow for a stealthier approach by the contact officer and put the element of surprise more in that officer's favor.

    "The less information the suspect can gather about the officer's location and angle, the slower he's likely to be in getting on target."

Lewinski stresses that "these are only tentative suggestions at this point. We are looking now to the training community for tactical strategies that can be tested with additional research."

The Force Science News is provided by The Force Science Research Center, a non-profit institution based at Minnesota State University, Mankato. Subscriptions are free and sent via e-mail. To register for your free, direct-delivery subscription, please visit www.forcesciencenews.com and click on the registration button.

(c) 2010: Force Science Research Center, www.forcescience.org. Reprints allowed by request. For reprint clearance, please e-mail: info@forcesciencenews.com. FORCE SCIENCE is a registered trademark of The Force Science Research Center, a non-profit organization based at Minnesota State University, Mankato.